what the world needs now is an
EMPATHY EPIDEMIC
by
GARY OLSON
____________________
Gary
Olson
chairs the Political Science Department at Moravian College
in Bethlehem, PA. He
is the author of Empathy
Imperiled: Capitalism, Culture, and the Brain
(New York: Springer-Verlag, 2013).
It’s
unarguable that human nature reveals a continuum of behaviors
ranging from the most wretched to the sublime, at times bordering
on the saintly. At a minimum, this means acknowledging, in the
words of Amin Maalouf that “There is a Mr. Hyde inside
each of us. What we have to do is prevent the conditions that
will bring the monster forth.”
At
the righteous end of the spectrum is our propensity for empathy,
a trait deeply rooted in our primate heritage. Empathy —
putting oneself in another’s emotional and cognitive shoes
and then acting appropriately — is now an incandescently
hot topic, virtually a cottage industry of books, articles and
YouTube videos.
Whereas
the evolutionary process has given rise to a hard-wired neural
system that equips us to connect with one another, many experts
believe that our empathically-impaired society needs nothing
less than an “empathy epidemic.” Among the factors
frequently cited as interfering with constructing an empathic
culture we find everything from parenting, education, and economic
inequality to early childhood programs, meaningful social connections,
and misplaced emphasis on achieving social status.
Dr.
Marco Iacaboni, one of the world’s recognized authorities
on the neuroscience of empathy, argues that the discovery of
mirror neurons, the neurons responsible for empathy, is “so
radical that we should be talking about a revolution, the mirror
neuron revolution.” Why? Because of the profound implications
for how we think about both individuals and the future of our
endangered planet. These neuroscience findings can be the foundation
for a fortuitous marriage between science and secular morality
but, as Prof. Iacoboni argues, this requires dissolving “the
massive belief systems that dominant our societies and that
threaten to destroy us.”
My
sense is that the most insidious, influential and largely unacknowledged
of these belief systems is neoliberal capitalist ideology. That
is, the critical missing piece in this lively and rapidly proliferating
conversation about empathy is the failure to identify the dynamic
convergence of culture, politics and the brain, what the eminent
political theorist William Connolly once describes as neuropolitics
or the “politics through which cultural life mixes into
the composition of the body/brain process. And vice versa.”
As
applied here, this explanation corresponds to what the French
philosopher Catherine Malebou has termed “neural ideology,”
the brain’s plasticity conforming to the social and political
organization of contemporary corporate capitalism. For me, the
most pertinent questions remain: how does this cultural information
gain access to the brain and what are the implications for understanding
the neuropolitics of empathy?
For
example, Dissident Voice readers are familiar with
research showing that empathic concern among college students
is strikingly lower than their 1970s counterparts and the decline
has been especially notable since 2000. But it’s far from
my intent to single out undergraduates for special censure.
It
can’t be emphasized too strongly that all brains are basically
alike, with the same equipment, but differing cultural experiences
contribute to shaping our brains, to how we think, including
how we think about empathy. Here I’m mindful not to caricature
Donald Hebb’s rule that “The neurons that fire together
wire together,” but his emphasis on the roles of repetition
and synaptic plasticity draws our attention to the critical
role of culture’s neurobiological imprinting.
Recent
compelling research within cultural neuroscience demonstrates
that specific, repetitive cultural priming has a measurable
influence on the brain and this neural signature begins in early
childhood. Tellingly, it can even override hardwired traits.
As such, and in Henry Giroux’s apt phrase, our dominant
empathy-anesthetizing, neoliberal culture has become the “public
pedagogy” that brackets off feelings of social solidarity
across the entire society.
If
an empathy deficit is more apparent among undergraduates it’s
because they are the legacy of over three decades of unrelenting
exposure to our neoliberal ideology of unfettered greed and
capitalism’s dominant narrative about human nature. Freedom
has been reduced to the pursuit of economic self-realization
and the self, a hyper-competitive, perpetual consumer, largely
indifferent to the fate of others and comfortable with the commodification
of morals. This cultural construction of the self is based primarily
on market values, leaving selective moral amnesia in its wake.
Lest
I be misunderstood here, our hegemonic culture’s social
engineering allows for and even encourages individual expressions
of empathy, including the volunteerism of philanthro-capitalists.
Because such acts only treat symptoms and not sources, they
are culturally sanctioned, pose no structural threats, and function
to attenuate the acceptance, legitimization and institutionalization
of social empathy on a grand scale.
Aside
from a few notable exceptions, empathy experts have failed to
unpack the political questions involved in investigating the
encultured brain. For example, to the extent that traditional
social science has explained culture as the neutral transmission
of beliefs, values, mores and laws, it doesn’t illuminate
the conscious, active invention of culture by institutions serving
particular class interests.
Demonstrating
how powerful groups seek to influence culture and how this impacts
the brain isn’t the daunting task one might imagine but
that hasn’t prevented mainstream intellectuals from rarely
if ever accepting this challenge. As such, the rapidly emerging
neurodisciplines that fail (or refuse) to account for class
will have, at best, no explanatory value and at worst, obfuscate
reality under the guise of value-free scientific inquiry. This
failure has devastating consequences in terms of understanding
the almost total muting of our empathic impulse.
Studies
on the evolutionary and biological origins of empathy are ongoing
but we now have hard empirical evidence, not wishful thinking
or even logical inference, on behalf of a case for organizing
vastly better societies. There is sufficient evidence that our
potential for empathic engagement is being subverted by the
dominant economic system and its ideology.
If
an ethos of caring is an essential part of what it means to
be human and an elemental requirement for human happiness, then
empathically impaired societies must be found wanting and challenged.
The tacit decision by neuroscholars to ignore or exclude this
hypothesis from the research agenda, debate and conversation
on empathy is inexcusable but not wholly unexpected.
Also
by Gary Olson:
Martin
Luther King's Dream in the Balance
Unmaking
War, Remaking Man
Rifkin
and Singer